- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Done deal—cohabiting dominant and subordinate dogs differently rely on familiar demonstrators in a detour task
BMC Biology volume 23, Article number: 125 (2025)
Abstract
Background
Companion dogs live in a mixed-species environment, where they can successfully learn from both humans and dogs. Breed type, the demonstrator’s behavior, and in multi-dog households, the dogs’ hierarchy are known influencing factors of the efficiency of dogs’ social learning. In previous studies, always an unfamiliar dog or experimenter was the demonstrator of the given task. Now we tested social learning in a setting more relevant to the everyday life of dogs, where the demonstrator was either the owner or a cohabiting dog. We used the validated dog-rank assessment questionnaire (DRA-Q) and the well-established detour paradigm. We hypothesized that beyond the previously found associations between social learning and rank, we would find stronger differences between high- and low-ranking cohabiting dogs due to the subjects’ everyday experience and different relationships with the demonstrators.
Results
We found that dominant dogs learn more effectively from the owner than from their subordinate dog companion. Subordinate dogs increased their success rate only when their dominant counterpart demonstrated the task, but did not improve when the owner was the demonstrator. Dogs with higher agonistic rank could improve their detour speed more often than the lower-ranked individuals in the Owner demonstration group, but we found no effect of the subranks in the Dog demonstration group.
Conclusions
These results warrant the intricate effect of within-group hierarchy of dogs even in non-competitive contexts. The strong difference between the subordinate and dominant dogs’ learning performance in the Owner-demonstration group aligns with the “owner as the main resource for dogs” hypothesis.
Background
The social nature of dogs is a basic species-specific feature, including both their relationship with their conspecifics and, importantly, with humans [1], and it is often emphasized in comparative studies between dogs and cats (for a review see [2]).Compared to the solitary wildcat, the hypothetical ancestor of dogs was the Paleozoic wolf, which had well-developed group-living habits [3]. Given the extensive knowledge about wolves’ complex social behavior [4], one might initially assume that dogs retain a similarly rich social behavioral repertoire [5]. However, empirical evidence suggests otherwise.
In reality, compared to wolves, the intraspecific social behavior of dogs shows only moderate complexity and a more ad hoc nature even in the case of the most relevant ecotype of the species, the village (or pariah) dogs [6]. Although often living in more-or-less stable connected groups, ownerless free-roaming dogs rarely show cooperative activities such as alloparental care [7], territorial defense [8, 9] or obtaining food [10]. Based on comparative studies with similarly raised and kept, group-living, socialized dogs and wolves, it was found that while the tame wolves showed similar willingness to cooperate with their conspecifics and humans, dogs preferred to cooperate mostly with humans and not with dogs [11]. Range and Virányi [12] argued that due to domestication, the early and fundamental change in the feeding ecology of the ancestral dog population resulted in selective pressure for increased interactivity and cooperativity with humans; meanwhile, within-species competition became stronger in dogs. Among free-roaming dogs (e.g., [8, 13]), an intricate system of dominance relationships was described, where the individual dog’s rank had various components. The most direct, linear hierarchy resulted from the agonistic dominance component [13]. An additional, affiliative–submissive component was connected to “formal” dominance status, which showed a more context-specific and less linear structure than the agonistic dominance component [13], but together with the high-ranking individual’s age, could also culminate in the higher “leadership” component [14]. Beyond the free-ranging dogs and other permanently or temporarily aggregated dog groups [15], intraspecific competitive and cooperative behaviors were rarely investigated among dogs kept under close human supervision (i.e., “owned” dogs) (see e.g., [16, 17]).
Recently we introduced a new theoretical perspective about the role of the owner/caretaker as the main—social—resource for companion and working dogs to compete over [18,19,20]. We argued that while the classical resources involved in competitive interactions (e.g., food, shelter, mating) are usually under strong and mostly egalitarian control by the dog owner, dogs would keep competing to monopolize the attention, affection, and interactions of their owner (as seen in the case of jealous behavior: [17]). Whenever animals live in temporarily stable groups, repeated competitive interactions may result in the formation of hierarchies [21]; therefore, we proposed that the within-group hierarchies among cohabiting companion dogs may also mirror their successful access to the owner [18]. In a recent study, we showed that the summary outcome of those everyday competitive interactions between cohabiting dogs, that the owners can readily notice, was in close association with an experimentally induced competitive scenario (toy possession), and other rank-related behaviors such as greeting between conspecifics upon reunion after a short isolation [20]. Importantly, the study from Vékony et al. [20] found that the separate components of dogs’ rank score (the so-called “subscores”), had a better explanatory effect regarding the competitive behaviors of the subjects. In particular, “Agonistic” and “Formal” dominance subscores aligned well with the differences between dogs’ resource holding and intraspecific greeting behaviors. However, it was still largely unknown (see rank-related “leading” behavior during walks, [22]) whether the rank position of dogs would affect such behaviors that are not directly associated with hierarchy; however, due to their social nature, they may refer to the social dynamics within the cohabiting dog-groups.
Social learning is considered as one of the main behavioral synchronizing mechanisms between dogs and humans [1]. There is ample evidence that dogs in general show an excellent capacity for copying human actions in various problem-solving tasks [23], including the “over-imitation” of otherwise unnecessary steps within a behavioral sequence seen from a human demonstrator [24], and showing preference for a socially reinforced strategy over a more direct solution that would require trial-and-error learning [25]. Interestingly, until now, relatively few factors have been found that would affect the performance of dogs in social learning tasks (e.g., lack of breed, and age-effect in a detour task: [26]), in other words, their capacity for learning from a con- or heterospecific demonstrator can be considered as being widespread, and more or less uniform. Even the familiarity of the demonstrator with the observers was found to have an ambiguous effect in social learning tasks, with studies that did [27] and, contrarily, did not find it [28] as having a strong influence on the observer dogs’ performance. So far, only a few factors have been described as relevant influencers of the dogs’ social learning capacity. Among them was the attention-eliciting communication from the human demonstrator [29, 30], and the selective past of particular dog breeds: “cooperative” working dogs learned more effectively from a human demonstrator than “independent” working dogs did [31]. Importantly, another factor with a strong effect was the hierarchy rank of dogs: Pongrácz et al. [32, 33] found that dominant dogs did not learn from a dog demonstrator; however, they were quite capable of learning from a human demonstrator. On the other hand, subordinate dogs effectively learned from both kinds of demonstrations. However, in that original experiment, dominant and subordinate dogs from different households were tested, and the demonstrator dog and human were both previously unknown individuals for the test subjects. This made it questionable whether the results in those earlier studies could be related to the within-group rank relationships of dogs. Furthermore, although modern companion dogs routinely meet with unknown humans and dogs (e.g., [34]), the hierarchy-based evaluation of dog behavior among unfamiliar individuals can raise some concerns with regard to biological relevance.
In our present research, we opted for the robust detour paradigm coupled with social learning, but at this time the observers and demonstrators were cohabiting dogs (conspecific learning scenario) and their owners (heterospecific learning scenario). First, we assessed the rank order of cohabiting dogs (using the validated Dog Rank Assessment questionnaire (DRA-Q) described by Vékony et al. [20]); then, we tested both for their individual problem-solving ability (detour without demonstration) and in a social learning context. In the latter case, dogs from the same household served as conspecific demonstrators for each other, and the dogs’ owner functioned as the human demonstrator.
We hypothesized that similar to earlier investigations [32, 33], dogs’ rank position will be in association with their efficiency in the social learning task. However, at this time we could formulate predictions based on the fact that the participants of the social learning task were familiar with each other. We predicted that dominant dogs will not pay attention to their subordinate partner, as was found earlier in the case of other species as well (chickens: [35]; vervet monkeys: [36]; also see opposite results with chimpanzees: [37]). Based on our hypothesis of “the owner as the main limited resource for the dog,” we predicted that dominant and subordinate dogs will differ in their motivation of paying attention and relying on their owner’s demonstrating action because the dominant dogs already secured their ability to access and closely follow the owner in everyday situations. On the other hand, we predicted that subordinate dogs will learn effectively from their dominant dog partner, as was suggested by other species examples (e.g., in deer mice: [38]).
Results
Only 35% of the dogs (29 out of 84) tested could solve the detour within 1 min in the first trial, and 24% (N = 20) of the dogs never made a successful detour: 27.6% of the dogs in the Control group (N = 8), 25% in the Dog demonstration group (N = 6) and 19.35% in the Owner demonstration group (N = 6).
Detour success
Cochran-Q test showed that dogs’ success improved across the trials only in the demonstration groups (Table 1): dogs performed better in the second and third trials than in the first one after the task was demonstrated by either the owner (Trial1–2: Z = − 2.67, p = 0.008; Trial1–3: Z = − 2.83, p = 0.005) or the cohabiting conspecific (Trial1–2: Z = − 2.83, p = 0.005; Trial1–3: Z = − 2.65, p = 0.008). No significant improvement was observed in the Control group (p1–2 = 0.317; p1–3 = 0.655). When analyzing subordinate and dominant dogs separately, we found that the success rate of subordinate dogs only improved across the trials in the Dog demonstration group (Trial1–2: Z = − 2, p = 0.046; Trial1–3: Z = − 2.24, p = 0.025), while dominant dogs more often succeeded after the demonstration in both demonstration groups (Owner: Trial1–2: Z = − 2.83, p = 0.005; Trial1–3: Z = − 2.71, p = 0.007; Dog: Trial1–2: Z = − 2, p = 0.046), although in the dog demonstration group there was no significant difference between the first and third trials (ptrial1–3 = 0.157, Fig. 1).
When we used the subranks, the Cochran-Q tests revealed a similar pattern to the one seen in the case of holistic rank (Table 1): Formally dominant dogs’ success rate only improved after owner demonstration (Trial1–2: Z = − 1.73, p = 0.083; Trial1–3: Z = − 2, p = 0.046), while subordinates only showed a trend-like improvement in the dog demonstration group (Trial1–2: Z = − 1.73, p = 0.083; Trial1–3: Z = − 1.73, p = 0.083, Fig. 2a).
Agonistically dominant dogs were more successful after the demonstration in both demonstration groups (Owner: Trial1–2: Z = − 2.83, p = 0.005; Trial1–3: Z = − 3.16, p = 0.002; Dog: Trial1–2: Z = − 2.24, p = 0.025; Trial1–3: Z = − 1.73, p = 0.083), while subordinates only succeeded more after the dog demonstration (Trial1–2: Z = − 1.73, p = 0.083; Trial1–3: Z = − 2, p = 0.046, Fig. 2b).
Dominant leaders succeeded more after both demonstrations (Owner: Trial1–2: Z = − 2.33, p = 0.020; Trial1–3: Z = − 2.53, p = 0.011; Dog: Trial1–2: Z = − 2, p = 0.046), although trial 3 did not differ from trial 1 in the dog demonstration group (ptrial1–3 = 0.157), while the followers’ success rates marginally improved after the other dog demonstrated the task twice (Trial1–3: Z = − 1.73, p = 0.083, Fig. 2c).
Latency of reaching the reward
Between-group differences
Based on the results of all dogs in our sample, we first analyzed if there was a difference in the improvement of dogs’ detour latencies between groups. We found an interaction between trial number and experimental group. Dogs in the Control group only improved in the third trial (Trial1–3: β ± SE = − 1.530 ± 0.50, z = − 3.05, p = 0.007), while in the Dog demonstration group, dogs made a detour significantly faster in both trials after demonstration than in the first trial (Trial1–2: β ± SE = − 2.348 ± 0.57, z = − 4.15, p < 0.001; Trial1–3: β ± SE = − 2.412 ± 0.59, z = − 4.07, p < 0.001). Their latencies did not differ between the second and third trials (p = 0.99). In the Owner demonstration group, there was a significant difference between each trials’ detour latencies, with steady improvement from trial 1 to trial 3 (Trial1–2: β ± SE = − 1.923 ± 0.45, z = − 4.25, p = 0.0001; Trial1–3: β ± SE = − 2.977 ± 0.48, z = − 6.20, p < 0.001; Trial2–3: β ± SE = − 1.054 ± 0.37, z = − 2.86, p = 0.012, Fig. 3).
Effects of rank within groups
Control group
When using the holistic rank and rank score in our analysis, in the Control group we only found a significant interaction between trials and the age of the dogs: older dogs in general were slower in solving the detour, but according to the post hoc test they improved more after the first trial (Trial1–2: β ± SE = − 0.621 ± 0.28, z = − 2.25, p = 0.062; Trial1–3: β ± SE = − 0.823 ± 0.30, z = − 2.76, p = 0.016).
When investigating the subranks and subscores separately, we found a similar interaction between trial and age (Trial1–2: β ± SE = − 0.591 ± 0.27, z = − 2.20, p = 0.071; Trial1–3: β ± SE = − 0.784 ± 0.28, z = − 2.76, p = 0.016). The best model also contained each of the three subranks, although their effects pointed in opposite directions: Formally dominant dogs made the detour faster than both the subordinate (β ± SE = 3.670 ± 1.07, z = 3.42, p = 0.002) and flexible ranked dogs (β ± SE = 5.370 ± 1.86, z = 2.89, p = 0.011). Agonistically dominant dogs, on the other hand, were slower than the subordinates (β ± SE = − 4.885 ± 1.32, z = − 3.69, p < 0.001) or flexible ranked dogs (β ± SE = − 4.430 ± 1.41, z = − 3.15, p = 0.005), and a similar effect was found in the case of Leadership rank (Dom-Sub: β ± SE = − 3.570 ± 1.29, z = − 2.76, p = 0.016; Dom-Flex: β ± SE = − 2.990 ± 1.22, z = − 2.45, p = 0.038). This model had a better fit than the one without subranks (∆AICc = 10.44, p < 0.001).
Owner demonstration
In the case of Owner demonstration, the holistic rank score did not have a significant effect; only the Trials showed a significant association with the detour latencies. Dogs improved not only between the first and second (β ± SE = − 1.820 ± 0.47, z = − 3.85, p < 0.001) and the first and third (β ± SE = − 2.920 ± 0.51, z = − 5.72, p < 0.001) but also between the second and third trials (β ± SE = − 1.090 ± 0.37, z = − 2.94, p = 0.009).
Trials had a similar effect in the models with subranks as an independent factor. Agonistic rank was in an interaction with trial: while agonistically dominant dogs improved in all trials (Trial1–2: β ± SE = − 2.573 ± 0.74, z = − 3.49, p = 0.001; Trial1–3: β ± SE = − 4.023 ± 0.78, z = − 5.18, p < 0.001; Trial2–3: β ± SE = − 1.450 ± 0.49, z = − 2.97, p = 0.009), subordinate dogs’ improvement was only significant from the first to the third trial (β ± SE = − 1.917 ± 0.65, z = − 2.97, p = 0.008). In their case, no significant difference was found between other trials (p1–2 = 0.13; p2–3 = 0.41, Fig. 4). This model had a better fit (∆AICc = 2.48, p = 0.027).
Dog demonstration
In the Dog demonstration group, only trial had a significant effect in all final models. Dogs solved the detour faster after the demonstration in both the second (β ± SE = − 2.875 ± 0.68, z = − 4.28, p < 0.001) and third trial (β ± SE = − 2.950 ± 0.71, z = − 4.19, p < 0.001) compared to the first.
Discussion
In this study, we used the well-established detour paradigm to investigate how rank relationships between cohabiting family dogs might affect their social learning ability from each other and the owner. While the possible effect of rank on social learning was previously investigated by Pongrácz and colleagues [32, 33], the demonstrators in those studies were either an unfamiliar experimenter or an unfamiliar dog, making the situation harder to interpret for everyday life situations. The other difference to previous social learning detour studies was that they usually only used the latency of the detour as a measure of improvement from trial to trial, but we decided to also use a much simpler measure: the binary variable of dogs’ success in reaching the treat.
Pongrácz and colleagues [28] mentioned in their first detour study that this task is difficult for dogs, and our results highlight this notion even more with the high proportion of unsuccessful dogs in every condition.
The success rates largely aligned with our predictions. Subordinate dogs obtained the reward with increasing success rates across the trials only when the other (dominant) dog was the demonstrator; meanwhile, their success rate did not change when their owner showed how to detour the fence. The dominant dogs, in general, performed better than the subordinate ones; however, in their case, the owner especially served as a salient demonstrator, and the dominant dogs’ improvement was only modest when they observed the demonstration of their lower-ranking companion. Regarding the success rates, the holistic rank and the three subscores had very similar associations with the dogs’ performance. The effect of the three subscores always pointed in the same direction, with only the “formally dominant” dogs showing no significant improvement in the case of the dog demonstrator. When we compare these results to the similar earlier study [32], the new findings mainly confirmed the earlier results. Although, in the first study, the researchers only analyzed the detour latencies, they also found that dominant dogs learned more successfully from the human demonstrator, while the subordinate animals mainly excelled after observing the demonstrator dog’s action.
Compared to the earlier study, we also found some differences. In the present experiment, higher-ranking dogs were not completely unable to learn from the other dog: although to a lesser extent than after observing their owner, they still improved after the dog demonstration. Even more strikingly, now we found that lower-ranking dogs were not improving at all in their success rates after observing the demonstration by their owner, while in previous studies, they successfully learned from a stranger [32]. As in our current test, detour latencies did not (or only minimally) associate with the rank of the dogs; the above-mentioned differences between the findings of Pongrácz et al. [32] and this study cannot be attributed solely to the different analyzed parameters, but rather to the familiarity of demonstrators.
The result that lower-ranking dogs did not learn from the owner at all, meanwhile the higher-ranking dog from the same household did, can be considered as the clearest indication of the"owner as a resource’ hypothesis thus far [18,19,20]. This idea strongly differs from the theory that places humans (i.e., the owner) within the hierarchy of dogs at home (e.g., reviewed by [39]). In our opinion, and according to others (e.g., [40, 41]), in the case of companion dogs, it is biologically not meaningful to model the dog–human relationship in the framework of competitive hierarchy establishment. Contrary to this, cohabiting dogs compete for exclusive access to the owner’s attention and care (see also: jealousy-like behaviors, [17]). From this aspect, in established rank systems, higher-ranking dogs may get used to the primary access and interactions with the owner, while lower-ranking individuals could learn to be more independent of the owner’s influence. In our test, if the lower-ranking dogs showed lower levels of human dependence, this can cause weaker performance in the social learning task, as was found in the case of those dog breeds that have been selected for independent working tasks [29, 31]. Using cohabitant dogs as subjects and the owner as a demonstrator, instead of randomly chosen dominant and subordinate dogs and an unfamiliar experimenter, as in the case of Pongrácz et al. [32], could result in the stronger contrast between the efficiency of learning from the demonstrators in our current experiment. While subordinate dogs may turn towards an unfamiliar person with interest and willingness to follow, the demonstration performed by their owners could evoke only minimal interest because of the effect of learned disadvantage against their dominant dog companion.
The most striking result of the earlier study run by Pongrácz et al. [32] was that dominant dogs did not learn from the (unfamiliar) demonstrator dog at all, how to effectively detour the fence. Now we found that higher-ranking dogs excelled in learning from the owner, and they also showed modest improvement by observing the familiar, lower-ranking dog. This result highlights the potential relevance of familiarity among demonstrators and observers in the case of intraspecific social learning in dogs. Demonstrator-observer familiarity is a core requirement in most social learning studies on various species (e.g., budgerigars, [42]; guppies, [43]; rats, [44]). Dogs can be regarded as an exception, as they were found to be flexible in how readily they accept unfamiliar humans and dogs as demonstrators [24, 28, 32, 45]. However, as our present study has shown, cohabitant dogs can learn to pay attention to each other at home, and this may cause even the higher-ranking individuals to learn from their familiar canine group members.
In another study, dogs’ rank was found to be associated with some of their personality traits [19]. It would be interesting to know whether dogs’ personality could explain the differences between high- and low-ranking dogs’ performance in the detour task. According to Vékony and colleagues’ findings [19], high-ranking dogs could be characterized by features such as persistence, focus, efficiency (belonging to Conscientiousness); curiosity (belonging to Openness); and activity, assertiveness (belonging to Extroversion). Although these characteristics could result in better problem-solving performance (e.g., [46]), we found a lack of rank effect in the control group. This suggests that the rank-related differences found in the other test groups could be caused by such confounders, as higher drive to explore, or generally better problem-solving capacity.
The results based on latencies partially confirmed the earlier findings. Detour latency in the owner demonstration group showed a similar association with the Agonistic score as did the previous study by Pongrácz and colleagues [32]. While higher-scoring dogs improved their detour speed right after the first demonstration, lower-ranking dogs only improved after two demonstrations. Interestingly, the rank assessment questionnaire of the earlier study only consisted of four questions, and the scoring of the questions put more weight on agonistic behaviors (i.e., the dog winning fights could not be categorized as subordinate), which might explain why the Agonistic subscale of the currently used DRA-Q shows more similarity with the previous results than Rank as a whole.
However, in the dog-demonstration group, we did not find any effect of the rank; instead, all dogs became faster through the repeated trials, showing a general (and expected) effect of social learning from the demonstrator (similar to the results of [30, 47]). Compared to the results of the present study, earlier it was found that the dominant dogs were not able to improve their detour latencies at all when another dog demonstrated the task, while the subordinate dogs performed the detour very fast right from the beginning [32]. The familiarity between the demonstrator and observer dog in this present experiment could result in somewhat higher attention towards the demonstration in the higher-ranking dogs, which, coupled with the relatively low sample size, could result in a disappearing rank-effect.
While in other studies the subranks had divergent associations and their relevance was strongly context-dependent [20, 48], here they point to the same direction in the case of success rate. This suggests that social learning might be one of the few contexts where all aspects of rank have similar relevance. Additionally, the non-competitive aspect of the detour task and the demonstrators’ action could also be the reason why the various components of dominance did not affect differently the dogs’ behavior.
So far, no previous study has shown that any intrinsic factor would affect dogs’ performance in the detour task without demonstration, apart from the spatial arrangement of the obstacle (“outward” detour vs. “inward”: [28]; straight fence vs. V-shaped one: [49]). There are indications in the literature that dogs’ sex could be a confounder in various problem-solving tasks that include training and interaction with humans (e.g., female dogs responded better to gestural commands than male dogs did: [50]; male dogs received higher “boldness” scores and initially performed better in working dog trials than females did: [51]). However, we did not find any association among our parameters with the dogs’ sex, which was possibly because our samples were well-balanced between the two sexes. Similar to previous studies, the holistic rank score did not show any association with detour latency in the control group. Subranks, however, seem to affect individual problem-solving in different ways. Formally dominant dogs were faster to solve the detour on their own, while agonistically dominant dogs and leaders seem to be slower. This result provides intriguing insight into the potential association between particular subrank components and spatial problem solving. The dominance-component “leadership” involves higher activity levels and alertness, and together with “agonistic” predicts a more assertive dog—however, these characteristics are seemingly of little use to effectively solve the detour task. This result fairly resembles the somewhat surprising findings of Dobos and Pongrácz [29, 31] which showed that dog breeds that were originally selected for independent work tasks were not more successful on their own in the detour task than the cooperative, more human-dependent breeds were. However, dogs with higher “formal dominance” scores are mainly characterized by receiving active submission from their peers. The relatively higher performance of these dogs in the control condition can possibly be the result of their generally higher experience in problem solving. Formal dominance can be regarded as the most “established” rank status because it is based on the other group members’ respect [13]. Such positions in the hierarchy can be the result of not only assertiveness and strength but also experience and age.
Limitations to the study
The main limitations of this study affect the dog demonstration group. This was our smallest group due to difficulties in training demonstrator dogs. The success rate was generally low in our sample (24% of dogs did not have any successful trials), leaving fewer possible demonstrator dogs in the subject pool. Some dogs that had successful trials lost interest in the task, so they could not be used as demonstrators for their cohabiting conspecifics. In previous studies with dog demonstrators [32, 52], the unfamiliar demonstrator dogs were specifically chosen to be easily trained to solve the detour task and stay motivated after multiple demonstrations, but in our case, the amount of training was limited and the motivation of the cohabiting dogs was non-modifiable.
This group also differed from the control and owner demonstration groups because not only the focal dog, but the (familiar) demonstrator also had a Rank score, and these scores were mutually interdependent with each other. Our understanding of Rank scores is that the differences between scores of cohabiting dogs describe the steepness of the hierarchy between them, but using the score differences instead of the Rank scores would make comparisons between the experimental groups impossible, as there are no such measures in the control and owner demonstration group.
Another limitation stems from the questionnaire itself. While previous studies have shown that formal submissive displays are the most unambiguous indicators of rank [13, 53], in our case, only one item of the DRA-Q refers to the formal rank. This, especially with the option of the “not applicable” response, made it impossible to use Formal score in the latency analyses.
Conclusions
Our findings suggest that social ranks in cohabiting dogs may have relevance in non-competitive scenarios such as learning from the owner and their cohabiting conspecifics. Contrary to previous findings, dominant dogs effectively learned from their cohabiting dog’s demonstration, although their success rate improved less when subordinate dogs observed the high-ranking dog demonstrator. This suggests that the familiarity of the demonstrator is an important factor in social learning from conspecifics even in the case of very socially flexible dogs. This effect was previously tested and has not been found in the case of human demonstrators.
The marked difference between the high- and low-ranking dogs’ improvement in their success rates after observing the owner demonstration suggests a qualitative, rank-related difference in motivation and relationship with the owner in the cohabiting dogs. This finding supports our notion that relationship with the owner—in the absence of other limited resources—might be the driving force behind forming hierarchies among cohabiting companion canines.
Methods
Subjects
Our subjects were companion dogs from multi-dog households (i.e., the owner had at least two dogs at home). The owners always participated with two of their dogs in our tests. The owners first completed the DRA-Q questionnaire that we used to determine the holistic Rank scores and Formal, Agonistic, and Leadership subscores of their dogs [20].
We tested Ntested = 95 dogs, but had to exclude some of them from the video coding (we excluded one subject due to the owner not following the protocol, two because of technical issues with the cameras, and five dogs did not even try the detour). We coded the videos of Ncoded = 87 dogs (40 males, 47 females, Mage ± SD = 5.75 ± 3.43), but excluded three dogs from the final analyses as they solved the detour in the first trial under 10 s, leaving little room for improvement in the subsequent trials. We included Nanalyzed = 84 dogs in the statistical analyses (Tables 2 and 3).
Experimental setup
The tests were conducted in an approximately 10 × 10 m, enclosed, outdoor area at a dog school in Budapest, Hungary. We always ran the tests when there was no other training activity in the dog school and no other dogs could approach the test area. The transparent V-shaped fence (made of wire mesh stretched over a light steel frame, 3 m each side, 1 m high) was set up so its sides formed an approximately 80° angle and there were at least 3 m between the ends of the wings to the closest border of the enclosure (Fig. 5).
The starting line was marked with two plastic cones 2 m from the corner of the fence, and a white plastic plate was placed at the inner corner of the fence, so any reward used during the tests would be more visible for the dogs.
The tests were recorded from both sides, with a wide-angle action camera (GoPro Hero4 or Lamax 9.1 W) and a regular camera (Canon Powershot SX432 or Sony HDR-CX450).
Procedure
The test protocol was based on previous studies of Pongrácz and colleagues [18, 28, 32]. The dogs were tested in three experimental groups: Control group, Owner demonstration group, and Dog demonstration group. Within these groups, we aimed to have a similar number of dominant and subordinate dogs, based on their Rank scores (compared to the other participating dog from the same household). We tested the two dogs of each owner on two separate occasions.
Control group
The test consisted of three identical trials: the experimenter (E) kept the dog on leash at the starting point while the owner (O) called the dog’s attention and showed a treat or a toy (based on the owner’s choice, whichever reward motivated the dog the most), then walked to the corner of the V-shaped fence while keeping the dog’s attention on the reward, and then placed the reward on the plate in the inner corner of the fence from above. Then the O walked back to the dog, took the leash from the E and released the dog with a release cue (e.g., “Go get it!”). The O was asked to use verbal encouragement throughout the trials, but they were not allowed to use any verbal or gestural commands related to directions (e.g., “Forward,” “Turn,” “Go around,” “Left,” “Right”). Each dog had 60 s from being released to obtain the reward. After obtaining the reward, or if the 60 s elapsed without a successful detour, the O called the dog back and put them on leash.
Owner demonstration group
The first trial was identical to that of the Control group, but in the second and third trials, the O, instead of dropping the reward to the plate from above, walked around one side of the fence while keeping the dog’s attention using ostensive verbal cues (e.g., “Look, here it is”), placed the reward on the plate and walked out along the other wing of the fence (i.e., walking a “heart-shaped” path during demonstration). The direction of the detour was chosen randomly. After the demonstration, the trials were run identically, as it was done before (i.e., releasing the dog, measuring the time).
Dog demonstration group
The demonstrator in this group was always the cohabitant dog of the tested dog (TD). The cohabitant dog previously participated in the detour test, either in the Control group or Owner demonstration group. Before testing, the TD was taken for a walk while the demonstrator dog (DD) was further trained to reliably detour around the fence. The training lasted until the demonstrator dog detoured around the fence without any stopping or hesitation in three consecutive trials. The maximum length of the training was 15 min. If the demonstrator dog could not be trained, the TD was reassigned to the Control or Owner demonstration group.
The first trial for the Dog demonstration group was identical to the one described in the case of the Control and Owner demonstration groups. In the second and third trials, both dogs stood on the starting line (they were both on leash and the E held them there) while the O called their attention, showing the reward and placing it on the plate in the corner from above. After walking back, the O released the DD, while encouraging the TD to pay attention to the detour. After the DD successfully obtained the reward and was put back on leash, the owner put another reward in the corner of the fence while calling the TD’s attention, then released the TD, who again, had 60 s to obtain the reward.
Behavioral coding
After synchronizing the videos of the two cameras, the tests were coded with BORIS (v 7.13.6 © Olivier Friard and Marco Gamba [54]). Table 4 shows the coded behaviors.
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were performed by using R statistical software (v4.3.0, R Development Core Team, 2021) in RStudio (2024.04.2 + 764, RStudio Team, Boston, MA, USA) with packages AICcmodavg, coxme, emmeans, glmmTMB, lme4, lmerTestm, MuMIN, performance, rstatix, survival, and survminer. We used the Cochran-Q test to assess whether the success rates of dominant versus subordinate dogs differed across the trials within each group. This test was chosen because it is specifically designed for analyzing categorical data and comparing the frequencies of success between multiple trials in the same subjects. By applying this test, we were able to determine if the performance of dogs, as categorized by dominance status, improved over the course of the trials in each group. We used Mixed Effects Cox Regression models to analyze the association between Latency and dogs’ rank. We also included the age of the dog as an independent variable in the initial models. We used AIC-based model selection to find the parsimonious model. We performed the model selection with the holistic Rank and Rank score, then the subscores and subranks separately, and finally, we compared the final models to find the best fit.
Data availability
All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article [and its supplementary information file Supplement_data_raw_Hierarchy_detour.xlsx].
Abbreviations
- DRA-Q:
-
Dog Rank Assessment Questionnaire
- SE:
-
Standard error
- DomMale:
-
Dominant male
- SubMale:
-
Subordinate male
- DomFemale:
-
Dominant female
- SubFemale:
-
Subordinate female
- E:
-
Experimenter
- O:
-
Owner
- TD:
-
Tested dog
- DD:
-
Demonstrator dog
References
Topál J, Miklósi Á, Gácsi M, Dóka A, Pongrácz P, Kubinyi E, et al. Chapter 3 The dog as a model for understanding human social behavior. Adv Stud Behav. 2009;39:71–116.
Pongrácz P, Lugosi CA. Cooperative but dependent-functional breed selection in dogs influences human-directed gazing in a difficult object-manipulation task. Animals. 2024;14(16):2348.
Marshall-Pescini S, Kaminski J. The Social Dog: History and Evolution. In: Kaminski J, Marshall-Pescini S, editors. The Social Dog: Behavior and Cognition. Cambridge: Academic Press; 2014. p. 3–33.
Mech D, Boitani L. Wolves Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. The University of Chicago Press Chicago and Landon; 2003.
Siniscalchi M, d’Ingeo S, Minunno M, Quaranta A. Communication in Dogs. Animals. 2018;8(8):131.
Bradshaw J, Rooney N. Dog social behavior and communication. In: Serpell J, editor. The Domestic Dog Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People. 2nd ed. Cambridge University Press; 2016. p. 133–59.
Paul M, Majumder SS, Nandi AK, Bhadra A. Selfish mothers indeed! Resource-dependent conflict over extended parental care in free-ranging dogs. R Soc Open Sci. 2015;2:150580.
Bonanni R, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. Pattern of individual participation and cheating in conflicts between groups of free-ranging dogs. Anim Behav. 2010;79(4):957–68.
Pal SK. Urine marking by free-ranging dogs (Canis familiaris) in relation to sex, season, place and posture. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2003;80(1):45–59.
Sarkar R, Sau S, Bhadra A. Scavengers can be choosers: A study on food preference in free-ranging dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2019;216:38–44.
Marshall-Pescini S, Schwarz JFL, Kostelnik I, Virányi Z, Range F. Importance of a species’ socioecology: Wolves outperform dogs in a conspecific cooperation task. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2017;114(44):11793–8.
Range F, Virányi Z. Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: The ‘Canine Cooperation Hypothesis.’ Front Psychol. 2014;5:1582.
Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P, Bonanni R, Natoli E. Dominance in relation to age, sex, and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behav Ecol. 2010;21(3):443–55.
Bonanni R, Cafazzo S, Valsecchi P, Natoli E. Effect of affiliative and agonistic relationships on leadership behaviour in free-ranging dogs. Anim Behav. 2010;79(5):981–91.
Daniels TJ. The social organization of free-ranging urban dogs. I. Non-estrous social behavior. Appl Anim Ethol. 1983;10(4):341–63.
Ostojić L, Clayton NS. Behavioural coordination of dogs in a cooperative problem-solving task with a conspecific and a human partner. Anim Cogn. 2014;17(2):445–59.
Abdai J, Baño Terencio C, Pérez Fraga P, Miklósi Á. Investigating jealous behaviour in dogs. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):8911.
Pongrácz P, Rieger G, Vékony K. Grumpy dogs are smart learners—the association between dog–owner relationship and dogs’ performance in a social learning task. Animals. 2021;11(4):961.
Vékony K, Prónik F, Pongrácz P. Personalized dominance – a questionnaire-based analysis of the associations among personality traits and social rank of companion dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2022;247:105544.
Vékony K, Pongrácz P. Many faces of dominance: the manifestation of cohabiting companion dogs’ rank in competitive and non-competitive scenarios. Anim Cogn. 2024;27(1):12.
Broom M, Koenig A, Borries C. Variation in dominance hierarchies among group-living animals: modeling stability and the likelihood of coalitions. Behav Ecol. 2009;20(4):844–55.
Ákos Z, Beck R, Nagy M, Vicsek T, Kubinyi E. Leadership and path characteristics during walks are linked to dominance order and individual traits in dogs. PLoS Comput Biol. 2014;10(1):e1003446.
Pongrácz P. Social learning in dogs. In: Kaminski J, Marshall-Pescini S, editors. The Social Dog: Behavior and Cognition. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2014. p. 249–93.
Huber L, Salobir K, Mundry R, Cimarelli G. Selective overimitation in dogs. Learn Behav. 2020;48(1):113–23.
Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Topál J, Csányi V. Interaction between individual experience and social learning in dogs. Anim Behav. 2003;65(3):595–603.
Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Vida V, Csányi V. The pet dogs ability for learning from a human demonstrator in a detour task is independent from the breed and age. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2005;90(3–4):309–23.
Fugazza C, Moesta A, Pogány Á, Miklósi Á. Social learning from conspecifics and humans in dog puppies. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):9257.
Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Kubinyi E, Gurobi K, Topál J, Csányi V. Social learning in dogs: The effect of a human demonstrator on the performance of dogs in a detour task. Anim Behav. 2001;62(6):1109–17.
Dobos P, Pongrácz P. You talkin’ to me? Functional breed selection may have fundamentally influenced dogs’ sensitivity to human verbal communicative cues. BMC Biol. 2024;22(1):183.
Pongrácz P, Miklósi Á, Timár-Geng K, Csányi V. Verbal attention getting as a key factor in social learning between dog (Canis familiaris) and human. J Comp Psychol. 2004;118(4):375–83.
Dobos P, Pongrácz P. Would you detour with me? association between functional breed selection and social learning in dogs sheds light on elements of dog-human cooperation. Animals. 2023;13(12):2001.
Pongrácz P, Vida V, Bánhegyi P, Miklósi Á. How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)? Anim Cogn. 2008;11(1):75–82.
Pongrácz P, Bánhegyi P, Miklósi Á. When rank counts - Dominant dogs learn better from a human demonstrator in a two-action test. Behaviour. 2012;149(1):111–32.
Smuts B. Social Behaviour among Companion Dogs with an Emphasis on Play. In: Kaminski J, Marshall-Pescini S, editors. The Social Dog: Behavior and Cognition. Elsevier; 2014. p. 105–130.
Nicol CJ, Pope SJ. The effects of demonstrator social status and prior foraging success on social learning in laying hens. Anim Behav. 1999;57(1):163–71.
Canteloup C, Cera MB, Barrett BJ, van de Waal E. Processing of novel food reveals payoff and rank-biased social learning in a wild primate. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):9550.
Watson SK, Reamer LA, Mareno MC, Vale G, Harrison RA, Lambeth SP, et al. Socially transmitted diffusion of a novel behavior from subordinate chimpanzees. Am J Primatol. 2017;79(6):e22642.
Kavaliers M, Colwell DD, Choleris E. Kinship, familiarity and social status modulate social learning about “micropredators” (biting flies) in deer mice. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005;58(1):60–71.
Yin S. Dominance versus leadership in dog training. Compend Contin Educ Vet. 2007;29(7):414–7, 432.
Bradshaw JWS, Blackwell EJ, Casey RA. Dominance in domestic dogs-useful construct or bad habit? J Vet Behav. 2009;4(3):135–44.
Schilder MBH, Vinke CM, van der Borg JAM. Dominance in domestic dogs revisited: Useful habit and useful construct? J Vet Behav. 2014;9(4):184–91.
Heyes C, Saggerson A. Testing for imitative and nonimitative social learning in the budgerigar using a two-object/two-action test. Anim Behav. 2002;64(6):851–9.
Reader SM, Kendal JR, Laland KN. Social learning of foraging sites and escape routes in wild Trinidadian guppies. Anim Behav. 2003;66(4):729–39.
Laland KN, Plotkin HC. Social learning and social transmission of foraging information in Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). Anim Learn Behav. 1990;18(3):246–51.
Range F, Viranyi Z, Huber L. Selective Imitation in Domestic Dogs. Curr Biol. 2007;17(10):868–72.
Miklósi Á, Turcsán B, Kubinyi E. Chapter 7 - The Personality of Dogs. In: Kaminski J, Marshall-Pescini S, editors. The Social Dog. San Diego: Academic Press; 2014. p. 191–222.
Range F, Virányi Z. Social learning from humans or conspecifics: differences and similarities between wolves and dogs. Front Psychol. 2013;4:868.
Vékony K, Bakos V, Pongrácz P. Rank-Related Differences in Dogs’ Behaviours in Frustrating Situations. Animals. 2024;14(23):3411.
Pongrácz P, Veres B. Dogs, demonstrators, and detours – does the shape of the obstacle matter? Anim Behav. 2025;223:123125.
D’Aniello B, Scandurra A, Alterisio A, Valsecchi P, Prato-Previde E. The importance of gestural communication: a study of human–dog communication using incongruent information. Anim Cogn. 2016;19(6):1231–5.
Svartberg K. Shyness–boldness predicts performance in working dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002;79(2):157–74.
Lugosi CA, Udvarhelyi-Tóth KM, Dobos P, Pongrácz P. Independent, but still observant—dog breeds selected for functional independence learn better from a conspecific demonstrator than cooperative breeds in a detour task. BMC Biol. 2024;22(1):245.
Trisko RK, Smuts BB. Dominance relationships in a group of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Behaviour. 2015;152(5):677–704.
Friard O, Gamba M. BORIS: a free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7(11):1325–30.
Acknowledgements
The authors are thankful to Bea Kutenics, Hanna Schneider and Blanka Veres for assisting with the tests; and Celeste R. Pongrácz, who proofread the manuscript.
Funding
Open access funding provided by Eötvös Loránd University. Péter Pongrácz, Petra Dobos, and Kata Vékony were supported by the Hungarian National Research, Development and Innovation Office (NKFIH, Grant # K143077). Péter Pongrácz and Petra Dobos were supported by the Morris Animal Foundation (D25 CA-039). Petra Dobos was supported by the University Research Fellowships (EKÖP-24–2-I-ELTE-394) of the Ministry for Innovation and Technology.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
PP and KV conceptualized the research. PP and KV put together the methodology. FP and PD validated the data. KV performed the formal analysis. KV, PD and FP conducted the investigation. PP and KV procured the resources. KV did the data curation. PP and KV wrote the main manuscript text, and KV prepared the figures. PP supervised the project. KV, PD and FP and did the project administration. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
We conducted the research in accordance with the Hungarian regulations on animal experimentation and guidelines for use of animals in research, as outlined by the Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour (ASAB). This fully non-invasive experimentation methodology was reviewed and approved by the National Scientific Ethical Committee on Animal Experimentation (Certificate number PE/EA/1204–4/2021). Companion dogs were always tested in the presence of their owners. Tests were reward-based and fully non-invasive; however, we informed the owners that they could interrupt or stop the experiments any time they felt that their dog experienced high levels of stress. (This never happened.)
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Pongrácz, P., Dobos, P., Prónik, F. et al. Done deal—cohabiting dominant and subordinate dogs differently rely on familiar demonstrators in a detour task. BMC Biol 23, 125 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12915-025-02232-9
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12915-025-02232-9